





Study with the several resources on Docsity
Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan
Prepare for your exams
Study with the several resources on Docsity
Earn points to download
Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan
Community
Ask the community for help and clear up your study doubts
Discover the best universities in your country according to Docsity users
Free resources
Download our free guides on studying techniques, anxiety management strategies, and thesis advice from Docsity tutors
The relationship between intercultural sensitivity, ethnocentrism, and intercultural communication apprehension. Intercultural sensitivity is defined as an individual's ability to understand and appreciate cultural differences, which is essential for successful intercultural communication. The document also discusses the developmental process of intercultural sensitivity and its relationship with ethnocentrism and intercultural communication apprehension. The study uses Pearson product-moment correlations and stepwise regression analyses to examine these relationships.
Typology: Study notes
1 / 9
This page cannot be seen from the preview
Don't miss anything!
Guo-Ming Chen, University of Rhode Island
As one of the dimensions of intercultural communication competence, intercultural sensitivity has shown significant impact on different culture-related variables. Among them, ethnocentrism and communication apprehension in the intercultural communication context remains unexplored. This study attempts to explore the relationship, if any, among the variables ethnocentrism, intercultural communication apprehension, and intercultural sensitivity. The survey research method was used to collect data from 432 students at a mid-sized university in the northeastern area of the United States. The results of Pearson product-moment correlations and stepwise regression analyses confirm the negative relationship between intercultural sensitivity and the two variables. Directions for future research in this line of study are discussed.
The importance of intercultural sensitivity has been emphasized by scholars from a variety of disciplines including Communication Studies, Education, and Psychology. Most have concluded that intercultural sensitivity is required for successful and productive communication between people from different cultural backgrounds (Chen & Starosta, 1997; Graf, 2004; Moran, Harris, & Moran, 2007; Olson & Kroeger, 2001; Rosen, Digh, Siinger, & Phillips, 2000). As Chen (2005) indicated, due to the rapid development of communication and transportation technology, globalization has brought together people of diverse cultures, ethnicities, geographies, and religions in every aspect of contemporary human life. Being sensitive to cultural differences becomes a critical ability to decrease ethnocentrism and parochialism and for being competent in intercultural or multicultural interactions. The intent of this study is to explore the relationship among the variables of intercultural sensitivity, ethnocentrism, and intercultural communication apprehension. Intercultural sensitivity can be defined as âan individualâs ability to develop emotion towards understanding and appreciating cultural differences that promotes appropriate and effective behavior in intercultural communicationâ (Chen & Starosta, 1997, p. 5). The concept was treated as one of the dimensions of intercultural communication competence. Intercultural communication competence comprises three aspects: cognitive, affective, and behavioral abilities; the cognitive ability is represented by intercultural awareness, the affective ability by intercultural sensitivity, and the behavioral ability by intercultural effectiveness or adroitness (Chen, 2009; Chen & Starosta, 1996, 2003, 2005; Hammer, 1989). According to Bennett (1984, 1986), intercultural sensitivity is a developmental process, in which individuals are able to transform themselves from the ethnocentric stage to ethnorelative stage. There are six stages involved in this transformational process. First, in the denial stage , the persons deny the existence of cultural differences; second, in the defense stage , the persons attempt to defend their own world views by facing the perceived threat; third, in the minimization stage , the persons protect their core values by concealing
differences under the cover of cultural similarities; fourth, in the acceptance stage , the persons begin to recognize and accept cultural differences in both cognitive and behavioral levels; fifth, in the adaptation stage , the persons develop sensitive and empathic skills to adapt to cultural differences and move into the bicultural or multicultural level; and finally, in the integration stage , the persons are able to establish an ethnorelative identity and enjoy the cultural differences. Thus, interculturally-sensitive persons have the ability to project and receive positive emotional responses before, during, and after interactions, which in turn leads to a higher degree of satisfaction and helps people achieve an adequate social orientation that enables them to understand their own and their counterpartsâ feelings and behaviors (Gudykunst & Kim, 2002). The positive emotional responses produced by intercultural sensitivity inevitably demonstrate an individualâs willingness to not only acknowledge and recognize, but also to respect and appreciate cultural differences during intercultural interaction (Bhawuk & Brislin, 1992; Chen, 2005). In other words, the acquirement of intercultural sensitivity refers to the absence of ethnocentrism and parochialism, which is a critical component for fostering successful global citizenship on both individual and organizational levels (Adler, 2008; Thorn, 2002). Chen and Starosta (2000) contended that intercultural sensitivity is one of the essential factors for intercultural communication consists of five abilities, including (a) interaction engagement, (b) respect for cultural differences, (c) interaction confidence, (d) interaction enjoyment, and (e) interaction attentiveness, that taken together form the dimensions of intercultural sensitivity. Chen and Starostaâs (2000) study indicated that individuals with high intercultural sensitivity tend to be more attentive, more able to perceive socio-interpersonal relationships in order to adjust their behaviors, to show high self-esteem and self-monitoring, more empathic, and more effective in intercultural interaction. The research findings of intercultural sensitivity indicate that the concept is very likely related to the other two communication traits (ethnocentrism and intercultural communication apprehension) which are central to understanding personal motivation and behavior in the process of intercultural communication. The purpose of this study then is to explore the potential impact of intercultural sensitivity on these two concepts. Ethnocentrism teaches individuals to bond tightly with their group members and to feel âproud of their own heritages by subjectively using their cultural standards as criteria for interpretations and judgments in intercultural communicationâ (Chen & Starosta, 2005, p. 27). For a culture to survive, a certain degree of ethnocentrism from its members is necessary; however, when ethnocentrism grows to a certain level, it will become a barrier for communication among people from different cultures (Neuliep & McCroskey, 1997a). Ethnocentrism leads persons to use their own group as the center of viewing things, which tends to develop prejudice by judging other groups as inferior to their own (Graen & Wakabayashi, 1994). According to Lin and Rancer (2003), while ethnocentrism might potentially bring about positive outcomes such as patriotism and willingness to sacrifice for oneâs own group, its negative consequences are obvious in intercultural communication, because individuals with a high level of ethnocentrism tend to misperceive people from other groups and misinterpret
RQ2. Is there a relationship between ethnocentrism and intercultural communication apprehension?
Method
Participants
Participants in this study were 432 undergraduate students (154 males, 275 females, and 3 missing data) in a mid-sized university in the northeastern area of the United States. The average age was 19.74 years.
Measures
Chen and Starostaâs (2000) Intercultural Sensitivity Scale, Neuliep and McCroskeyâs (1997b) Generalized Ethnocentrism scale (GENE), and Neuliep and McCroskeyâs (1997a) Intercultural Communication Apprehension scale were used in this study to measure the three concepts. The 24-item Intercultural Sensitivity scale contains five dimensions: interaction engagement, respect of cultural differences, intercultural confidence, interaction enjoyment, and intercultural attentiveness. The dimension of interaction engagement concerns the participantsâ feeling of participation in the process of intercultural communication. Respect for cultural differences refers to how participants orient to or tolerate cultural differences in their counterparts. Interaction confidence indicates the participantsâ degree of confidence during the intercultural interaction. Interaction enjoyment deals with participantsâ reaction to communication that is culturally different. Interaction attentiveness reflects participantsâ efforts to understand what is going on in intercultural communication. The validity and reliability of the scale has shown stable validity and reliability in different cultural contexts (Fritz, Hentze, Mollenberg, & Chen, 2005; Fritz, Mollenberg, & Chen, 2002; Peng, 2006; Yu & Chen, 2008). The alpha coefficient of the scale was .86 in the original study and .88 in this study. The 18-item GENE scale was used to measure the degree of participantsâ ethnocentrism. The original Cronbachâs alpha was .92 and .80 in this study. The 14-item Neuliep and McCroskeyâs (1997a) Intercultural Communication Apprehension scale was used to measure the degree of participantsâ apprehension in intercultural context. The original Cronbachâs alpha was .92 and .87 in this study.
Analysis
All measuring instruments used in this study are 5-point Likert scales. In order to examine the relationship among intercultural sensitivity, ethnocentrism, and intercultural communication apprehension (i.e., H1, H2, and RQ2), Pearson product-moment correlations were conducted. To answer RQ1 stepwise regression analyses were conducted.
Table 1. Pearson Correlation Coefficients for Variables Variables IS Ethno ICA Intercultural Sensitivity (IS)
-- -.54* -.60*
Ethnocentrism (Ethno) -- .28* Intercultural Communication Apprehension (ICA)
--
Note. N = 432. *p<.
Table 2. Summary of Stepwise Multiple Regression Analyses
R R^2 F P beta
Intercultural Engagement
.51 .26 152.57 .01.
2.Intercultural Communication Apprehension Cultural Differences
.67 .45 351.09 .01 -.
Intercultural Enjoyment
.73 .54 351.09 .01 -.
Note. N = 432.
Results
H1 predicted that a negative relationship exists between intercultural sensitivity and ethnocentrism. It was found that a significant correlation exists between the two variables at the p < .01, r = .-54. H2 predicted that a negative relationship exists between intercultural sensitivity and intercultural communication apprehension. The result showed that a significant correlation exists between the two variables at the p < .01, r = -.60. The result for the RQ2 showed that there is a positive correlation between ethnocentrism and intercultural communication apprehension (r = .28, p < .01). Table 1 reports the results of the correlations. Stepwise regression analyses were conducted to find out which of the five dimensions of intercultural sensitivity best predicted ethnocentrism and intercultural communication apprehension as specified in RQ1. Each of the five dimensions was regressed into the variables separately. Results indicated that ethnocentrism was best predicted by intercultural engagement (R = .51, R2 = .26, F = 152.57, P < .01, Beta = -.51), and intercultural communication apprehension was best predicted by respect of cultural differences (R = .67, R2 = .45, F = 351.09, P < .01, Beta = -.50) and intercultural enjoyment (R = .73, R2 = .54, F = 351.09, P < .01, Beta = -.34). Table 2 summarizes the result of the stepwise regression analyses.
Bennett, M. J. (1986). A developmental approach to training for intercultural sensitivity. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 10 , 179-196. Berger, C. R., & Calabrese, R. J. (1975). Some explorations in initial interaction and beyond: Toward a developmental theory of interpersonal communication. Human Communication Research, 1 , 99-112. Bhawuk, D. P. S., & Brislin, R. (1992). The measurement of intercultural sensitivity using the concepts of individualism and collectivism. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 16 , 413-436. Chen, G. M. (2005). A model of global communication competence. China Media Research, 1 , 3-11. Chen, G. M. (2009). Intercultural effectiveness. In L. A. Samovar, R. E. Porter, & E. R. McDaniel (Eds.), Intercultural communication: A reader (pp. 393-401). Boston: Wadsworth. Chen, G. M., & Starosta, W. J. (1996). Intercultural communication competence: A synthesis. Communication Yearbook, 19 , 353-384. Chen, G. M., & Starosta, W. J. (1997). A review of the concept of intercultural sensitivity. Human Communication, 1 , 1-16. Chen, G. M., & Starosta, W. J. (2000). The development and validation of the intercultural sensitivity scale. Human Communication, 3 , 1-15. Chen, G. M., & Starosta, W. J. (2003). A review of the concept of intercultural awareness. In L. A. Samovar & R. E. Porter (Eds.), Intercultural communication: A reader (pp. 344-353). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Chen, G. M., & Starosta, W. J. (2005). Foundations of intercultural communication. Lanham, MD: University Press of America. Fritz, W., Graf, A., Hentze, J., Möllenberg, A., & Chen, G. M. (2005). An examination of Chen and Starostaâs model of intercultural sensitivity in Germany and United States. Intercultural Communication Studies, 14 (1), 53-64. Fritz, W., Mollenberg, A., & Chen, G. M. (2002). Measuring intercultural sensitivity in different cultural context. Intercultural Communication Studies, 11 (2), 165-176. Furnham, A. (1987). The adjustment of sojourners. In Y. Y. Kim & W. B. Gudykunst (Eds.), Cross-cultural adaptation: Current approaches (pp. 42-61). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Graen, G. B., & Wakabayashi, M. (1994). Cross-cultural leadership makings: Bridging American and Japanese diversity for team advantage. In C. Triandis, M. D. Dunnette, & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 415-446). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Graf, A. (2004). Screening and training intercultural competencies: Evaluating the impact of national culture on intercultural competencies. International Journal of Human Resource Management, 15 (6), 1124-1148. Gudykunst, W. B. (1995). Anxiety/uncertainty management (AUM) theory. In R. L. Wiseman (Ed.), Intercultural communication theory (pp. 8-58). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Gudykunst, W. B. (2005). Theories of intercultural communication. China Media Research, 1 , 61-75.
Gudykunst, W. B., & Kim, Y. Y. (2002). Communicating with strangers: An approach to intercultural communication. New York: McGraw-Hill. Gudykunst, W. B., & Nishida, T. (2001). Anxiety, uncertainty, and perceived effectiveness of communication across relationships and cultures. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 25 , 55-72. Hammer, M. R. (1989). Intercultural communication competence. In M. K. Asante & W. B. Gudykunst (Eds.), Handbook of international and intercultural communication (pp. 247-260). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Kassing, J. W. (1997). Development of the intercultural willingness to communicate scale. Communication Research Reports, 14 (4), 399-407. Khan, Z., & Wiseman, R. L. (2007). What are you going to take from us now?: Perceived threats from immigrants, ethnocentrism, and intercultural communication apprehension. Intercultural Communication Studies, 16 (2), 55-70. Kim, Y. Y. (1988). Communciation and cross-cultural adaptation. Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters. Kim, Y. Y. (1995). Cross-Cultural adaption: An integrative theory. In R. L. Wiseman (Ed.), Intercultural communication theory (pp. 170-193). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Lin, Y., & Rancer, A. S. (2003). Ethnocentrism, intercultural communication apprehension, intercultural willingness-to-communicate, and intentions to participate in an intercultural dialogue program: Testing a proposed model. Communication Research Reports, 20 (1), 62-72. Lukens, J. G. (1978). Cross-cultural speech: Its nature and implications. Ethnic Groups, 2 , 35-
Ma, R. (2005). Communication between Hong Kong and Mainland Chinese: Rethinking cross-cultural adaptation. International and Intercultural Communication Annual, 28 , 197-213. McCroskey, J. C., & Richmond, V. P. (1987). Willingness to communicate. In J. McCroskey & J. A. Daly (Eds.), Personality and interpersonal communication (Vol. 6, pp. 129- 156). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. McCroskey, J. C., & Richmond, V. P. (1991). Willingness to communicate: A cognitive view. In M. Booth-Butterfield (Ed.), Communication, cognition, and anxiety (pp. 19-37). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Moran, R. T., Harris, P. R., & Moran, S. V. (2007). Managing cultural differences: Global leadership strategies for the 21st century. New York: Elsevier. Neuliep, J. W., & McCroskey, J. C. (1997a). The development of intercultural and interethnic communication apprehension scales. Communication Research Reports, 14 (2), 145-
Neuliep, J. W., & McCroskey, J. C. (1997b). The development of a U.S. and generalized ethnocentrism scale. Communication Research Reports, 14 (4), 385- Neuliep, J. W., & Ryan, D. J. (1998). The influence of intercultural communication apprehension and socio-communicative orientation on uncertainty reduction during initial cross-cultural interaction. Communication Quarterly, 46 (1), 88-99.