Docsity
Docsity

Prepare for your exams
Prepare for your exams

Study with the several resources on Docsity


Earn points to download
Earn points to download

Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan


Guidelines and tips
Guidelines and tips

Implications for Mathematics Teaching and Learning, Lecture notes of Mathematics

The purpose of this paper is to delineate the trajectory of fundamental learning theories and the way these theories have impacted the teaching and learning ...

Typology: Lecture notes

2021/2022

Uploaded on 09/12/2022

sunday
sunday 🇬🇧

4.5

(12)

213 documents

1 / 18

Toggle sidebar

This page cannot be seen from the preview

Don't miss anything!

bg1
2013 Mathitudes 1
Iman C. Chahine, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA
Delineating the Epistemological Trajectory of Learning Theories:
Implications for Mathematics Teaching and Learning
The purpose of this paper is to delineate the trajectory of fundamental learning theories and the
way these theories have impacted the teaching and learning of mathematics over more than half
a century. We argue that a critical examination of the depiction of learning theories and their
inherent implications for the teaching of mathematics afford an understanding of the
hierarchical evolution of the field of mathematics education. Needless to say, examining various
learning theories in the mathematics education context unpacks epistemological and
ontological core issues underlying the teaching and learning of vital topics that are assumed to
account for the changes in global economics and business in a STEM world.
Introduction
The quest to explore how people
learn mathematics has been a perennial
concern for decades. For some time now,
philosophers and learning theorists have
been incessantly searching for optimal
conditions under which learning occurs. A
tremendous bulk of contesting theories has
evolved, thriving to explain the roles of the
learner and the teacher when engaged in acts
of learning and teaching. Notwithstanding
the fact that many learning theories differ in
their empirical manifestations, nonetheless
the underlying epistemologies are
unequivocally analogous.
At the turn of the 21st century, the
necessity to prepare children for a rapidly
changing world has been consistently
reiterated in the literature (Csikszentmihalyi
& Schneider, 2000; Cornell & Hartman,
2007; Drucker, 2010). With the tumultuous
political and economic climate prevailing
worldwide exacerbated by challenges
emerging in the rapidly changing context of
new technologies and globalization, there is
an unprecedented need to capitalize on how
best to educate future generations. A critical
examination of the trajectories that various
theories of learning have taken since the last
century provides a clear explanation on the
fundamental problems facing education
locally and globally. In a highly dynamic
and versatile world, unfolding the best
practices particularly for teaching
mathematics and sciences instigates a
pressing need for consideration by educators
and policy makers. For mathematics
education, the push toward standards-based
curricula governed by accountability and
teaching effectiveness dogmas juxtaposed
with policies to enact cutting edge
educational interventions in an ever-
changing economy command inevitable
challenges that cannot be overlooked. The
highly political dynamics that depict the way
standards are drafted and sanctioned in
terms of content and pedagogy are
determining the way educational policies are
institutionalized and eventually endorsed at
the grass roots level.
Nevertheless, recent studies on
student motivation, attitudes and self-
efficacy have shown that unless students are
cognitively and emotionally invested in
what they are learning, little knowledge will
be acquired (Friedman, 2006; Furner &
Gonzalez-DeHass, 2011). Naturally,
designing learning environments that can
trigger students natural curiosity and
stimulate their interest will significantly
pf3
pf4
pf5
pf8
pf9
pfa
pfd
pfe
pff
pf12

Partial preview of the text

Download Implications for Mathematics Teaching and Learning and more Lecture notes Mathematics in PDF only on Docsity!

Iman C. Chahine, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA

Delineating the Epistemological Trajectory of Learning Theories:

Implications for Mathematics Teaching and Learning

The purpose of this paper is to delineate the trajectory of fundamental learning theories and the way these theories have impacted the teaching and learning of mathematics over more than half a century. We argue that a critical examination of the depiction of learning theories and their inherent implications for the teaching of mathematics afford an understanding of the hierarchical evolution of the field of mathematics education. Needless to say, examining various learning theories in the mathematics education context unpacks epistemological and ontological core issues underlying the teaching and learning of vital topics that are assumed to account for the changes in global economics and business in a STEM world.

Introduction The quest to explore how people learn mathematics has been a perennial concern for decades. For some time now, philosophers and learning theorists have been incessantly searching for optimal conditions under which learning occurs. A tremendous bulk of contesting theories has evolved, thriving to explain the roles of the learner and the teacher when engaged in acts of learning and teaching. Notwithstanding the fact that many learning theories differ in their empirical manifestations, nonetheless the underlying epistemologies are unequivocally analogous. At the turn of the 21st^ century, the necessity to prepare children for a rapidly changing world has been consistently reiterated in the literature (Csikszentmihalyi & Schneider, 2000; Cornell & Hartman, 2007; Drucker, 2010). With the tumultuous political and economic climate prevailing worldwide exacerbated by challenges emerging in the rapidly changing context of new technologies and globalization, there is an unprecedented need to capitalize on how best to educate future generations. A critical examination of the trajectories that various theories of learning have taken since the last century provides a clear explanation on the

fundamental problems facing education locally and globally. In a highly dynamic and versatile world, unfolding the best practices particularly for teaching mathematics and sciences instigates a pressing need for consideration by educators and policy makers. For mathematics education, the push toward standards-based curricula governed by accountability and teaching effectiveness dogmas juxtaposed with policies to enact cutting edge educational interventions in an ever- changing economy command inevitable challenges that cannot be overlooked. The highly political dynamics that depict the way standards are drafted and sanctioned in terms of content and pedagogy are determining the way educational policies are institutionalized and eventually endorsed at the grass roots level. Nevertheless, recent studies on student motivation, attitudes and self- efficacy have shown that unless students are cognitively and emotionally invested in what they are learning, little knowledge will be acquired (Friedman, 2006; Furner & Gonzalez-DeHass, 2011). Naturally, designing learning environments that can trigger students’ natural curiosity and stimulate their interest will significantly

impact their learning. Thompson and Thornton (2002) found that when students are intrinsically motivated they are eager to learn. Yair (2000) argues that, “students’ interest in what they learn, and their sense of enjoyment while learning, are highly correlated with the outcomes of learning” (p. 193). Furner and Berman (2003) found that teachers need to do more in the way they teach math to address attitudes toward math and mathematics anxiety in the classroom. However, research has suggested that motivation of adolescents decline as they progress through junior and senior high school (Eccles, Midgley, et al., 1993; Gonzalez, 2002; Hidi & Harackiewicz, 2000; Williams & Stockdale, 2004). Additionally, the value children place on many academic activities, particularly mathematics and their beliefs about the usefulness of school decline as they get older (Wigfield, Eccles, & Rodriquez, 1998). Today, students are more often described as “physically present but psychologically absent” and thus are less likely to actively and enthusiastically engage in learning. Recently, student lack of motivation in the mathematics classroom has been a critical national concern in light of efforts to improve STEM (Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics) education in the United States. One concern in particular is that the pipeline of students entering STEM does not meet the current demand for future scientists and engineers. One of the reasons attributed to the attrition rate of students embarking on a degree in STEM is students’ underperformance in high school mathematics and their inadequate preparation in rigorous mathematics content. Hence, addressing this national need requires research and development of the best pathways to remediate the teaching of mathematics and

thus provide sufficient support to students in learning the content. This leads us to the following questions: How did our understanding of how students learn develop in light of widened exposure to subsequent research efforts in educational and cognitive psychology? And what are the major learning theories that have impacted frequent paradigm shifts in the field of mathematics education? The purpose of this paper is to delineate the trajectory of fundamental learning theories and the way these theories have impacted the teaching and learning of mathematics over more than half a century. We argue that a critical examination of the depiction of learning theories and their inherent implications for the teaching of mathematics affords an understanding of the hierarchical evolution of the field of mathematics education. Needless to say, examining various learning theories in the mathematics education context unpacks epistemological and ontological core issues underlying the teaching and learning of vital topics that are assumed to account for the changes in global economies. Drawing on extensive literature related to learning theories that have emerged in the past two centuries, and examining critically possible formulations on how mathematical knowledge and problem solving activity can be constructed, we highlight important implications that, we argue, provide the most compelling explanation of how students learn mathematics. By virtually appealing to research on the cognitive as well as the social construction of mathematical knowledge, we explore various trends in teaching and learning practices proposed by each perspective and investigate their practical significance and implications in the mathematics classroom.

seemingly cumulative and hierarchical such that each type is built on its prerequisite, the highest in the hierarchy being problem- solving or type 8. The hierarchy includes: Problem-solving ; Principle learning ; Concept learning ; Multiple discrimination learning ; Verbal associations ; Stimulus- response learning, and Signal learning. Gagné also argued that for any learning to take place at any level special attention should be given to its prerequisites. Advocating the claims that the whole (goal or outcome) is the sum of its parts and that learning is rather specific and goal-directed, Gagné (1965) established task analysis as a technique that pinpoints conditions or behavioral objectives under which learning of a certain goal occurs. With a highlighted emphasis on “outcome content” (what to learn) rather than the process (how to learn), instruction was perceived to proceed from prerequisite skills to desired goals. Other important personal international factors such as motivation and establishment of attitudes and beliefs are ignored in favor of achieving behavioral objectives. Many drawbacks of Gagné’s guided approach to learning have been frequently cited in the literature, such as limited transfer and neglecting other important forms of learning such as informal and discovery learning (Post, 1988; Tuckman, 1992). On the other hand, theorists who have given more weight to cognitive explanations and mental structures are retained near the cognitive end of the continuum and are referred to as Cognitivists (Byrnes, 1992). Cognitivists such as Piaget, Bruner, and Dienes see knowledge as actively constructed by the learner in response to his/her interaction with the environment. In contrast to Neobehaviorists, Cognitivists emphasize the “how” of learning rather than the “what”. In Cognitivists’ views, the learner, an active

part in the learning process, uses internal mental structures to organize, transform and retrieve information when acting on a learning environment (Chahine, under review). As such, learning is intrinsic, holistic and personal to the individual and can be enhanced through interactions with others and with proper physical materials in the environment. A closer look at the works of these theorists merits our discussion of learning theories.

Piaget's Developmental Theory A prominent Cognitivist in the 20th century is the Swiss biologist and psychologist Jean Piaget (1896-1980). Piaget is renowned for constructing a highly influential model of intellectual development and learning. Piaget's developmental theory is based on the idea that the child builds cognitive mental structures in an attempt to make sense of his or her environment (Piaget, 1952). These stages, at least for Swiss school children, increase in sophistication with development and include: Sensorimotor stage (birth - 2 years old) where the child, through physical interaction with his or her environment, builds a set of concepts about reality and how it works. Preoperational stage (ages 2- 7) in which the child is not yet capable of abstract conceptualization and therefore needs exposure to concrete physical situations. The primary deficiency at this stage is what Piaget (1952) calls the reversibility principle where the child cannot grasp the idea of conservation of quantity. Concrete operations (ages 7-11) where physical experience accumulates, the child starts to conceptualize, creating logical structures that explain his or her physical experiences. He further claims that abstract problem-solving is also possible at this stage. Although a child’s actions are internalized and reversible, Piaget (1952)

explains that the child cannot deal with possibilities that are outside the realm of his/her direct experience, and finally formal operations (beginning at ages 11-15) where the child is capable of constructing formal operational definitions and thus, abstract concepts One of the significant implications of Piaget’s developmental theory is related to teaching basic concepts. A direct assumption is that a child should be helped to progressively proceed from the concrete to the more abstract modes of thought. Much of the mathematics taught following traditional curricula contradicts this assumption, through teaching by telling formal explanations of concepts are presented to the child in a different mode of thought than his own. In adapting to his/her environment thus avoiding and minimizing mathematics anxiety, Piaget (1977) contends that individuals use two mechanisms: Assimilation and accommodation. Assimilation , which involves incorporating new ideas into existing schemata in Piagetian theory, is somehow similar to the Behaviorists’ concept of stimulus where having learned to respond to one stimulus makes it easy to respond to another similar stimulus. Accommodation , on the other hand, involves modifying existing schemata to fit the newly assimilated information. Striking a balance or equilibrium between both assimilation and accommodation is the basis of intellectual development. Additionally, Piaget (1952) views intelligence as “adaptation to new circumstances” ( p.151) and explains that in any intelligent act “ the need which serves as motive power not only consists in repeating , but in adapting , that is to say, in assimilating a new situation to old schemata and in accommodating these schemata to new circumstances” (Piaget, 1952, p. 182).

Thus, Piaget does not view intelligence in terms of content or amount of knowledge, but rather as an arrangement or structure and a way in which information is organized. Other developmental factors that contribute to cognitive development include maturation, active experience social interaction, and equilibration or self- regulation. A basic tenet of learning by discovery or exploration requires a vigorous exposure to and involvement with the environment. A direct implication of Piaget’s theory is the focus on development of schemata that potentially facilitates problem-solving. The developmental stages delineated by Piaget are highly relevant to the teaching and learning of mathematical concepts. For example, at the primary level, students can be taught conservation problems in late elementary and early middle level through tasks involving seriation and classification. Moreover, structuring the physical environment by making multiple learning centers where students can be actively and purposefully involved in the learning process is necessary to enhance students’ attitudes toward math and motivation. In addition, utilizing hands- on activities with varieties of manipulatives and multiple physical embodiments helps children learn operations appropriate to their level of development. Piaget’s theory calls for more emphasis on integrative themes, like probability and statistics in early grades as well as inclusion of basic algebra concepts in primary grades, the case we see now in Common Core State Standards (National Governors Association Center for Best Practices (NGA Center) and the Council of Chief State School Officers (CCSSO) (2010). Engaging in hand-on explorations focused on these topics reinforces students’ beliefs in the relevance of mathematics to real life situations thus

Structuralists contend that the sum total of mental structures and their interactions comprise the conscious experience. A major implication of this theory is the role that immediate experiences and reflection play in stimulating complex perceptions and bolster students’ positive attitudes toward math thus invoking learning (Carlson, 2010). Structuralism as a movement lost its popularity in the 1960s with the emergence of post-structuralism, a French movement that critiqued the basic tenets of structuralism and called for social constructionism as a means to expose “subjugated knowledges” (Foucault, 2003). In principle, Structuralism and Functionalism symbolize two end-poles of a continuum vis à vis the degree of emphasis each paradigm merits either conceptual or procedural knowledge. Rittle-Johnson, Siegler, and Alibali (2001) defined procedural knowledge as “…the ability to execute action sequences to solve problems” (p. 346). They also argued that procedural knowledge involved mainly the use of previously learned step-by-step techniques and algorithms to solve specific types of problems. Furthermore, the authors explained that conceptual knowledge entails “… implicit or explicit understanding of the principles that govern a domain and of the interrelations between units of knowledge in a domain” (p.346). A handful of research studies support the hypothesis that forming correct problem representations is one mechanism linking improved conceptual knowledge to improved procedural knowledge (Jitendra, 2002; Hoffman & Spatariu, 2008). Rittle- Johnson, Siegler, and Alibali (2001) defined problem representation as “the internal depiction or re-creation of a problem in working memory during problem-solving” (p. 348). Furthermore, a number of studies investigated the role that problem

representation played in changing performance either positively or negatively, depending on the circumstances (Kohl & Finkelstein, 2005). While proponents of structuralism capitalize on the nature and organization of concepts, functionalists focus on how the mind operates in the course of problem- solving and information processing (Byrnes, 2007). Millroy (1992) defined the Piagetian approach as “the structural developmental” approach for it specifically concentrates on individual cognitive development, with little emphasis on social influences.

Problem Solving Theory Most of the approaches to problem solving established during the past thirty years practically fall under one of the two contradictory treatments: The information processing model of human thinking (Newell & Simon, 1972) and the social practice theory (Turner, 1982). The information processing model aims at describing the general processes of problem- solving, minimizing the role of individuals in the problem-solving process (Putnam et al., 1989). Schoenfeld (1983) extended by far the scope of traditional conception of mathematical problem- solving highlighting four fundamental dimensions of good practice: resources of mathematical knowledge, heuristic strategies, control over the process of working on problems, and a deep understanding of the nature of mathematical argumentation. Current interest in problem-solving as a “practice” reflects a trend in which learners are characterized as more active and where problem-solving is viewed as a series of activities (Lave et al., 1990). As a matter of fact, it has been widely argued that emphasizing the value of teaching problem solving in schools as a mathematical practice in contrast to rote procedural

approaches enables the learner to gain more in-depth understanding of mathematical principles underlying such practice (Nunes et al., 1993). The demand to integrate the learner with the surrounding environment, practice and culture is an approach that is embraced by the social practice theorists. In principle, the social practice theory seeks to interpret the meaning of social activity in a number of environments and to discern their causes. A close akin of the social practice theory is the Activity theory. Activity theory has been widely employed in socio-cultural and socio-historical research that aimed to scrutinize human activity systems (Jonassen & Murphy, 1999). It has been extensively used as a framework for developing Constructivist Learning Environments (CLE) and cognitive tools for learning. The assumptions of activity theory resonate with those of constructivism, situated learning, distributed cognitions, case-based reasoning, social cognition, and everyday cognition. Activity theory has its roots in the Soviet cultural-historical psychology of Vygotsky, Leont'ev (1978) and it represents an alternative perspective to the claim that learning must precede activity. The most fundamental assumption of activity theory is that the “human mind emerges and exists as a special component of interactions with the environment, so activity (sensory, mental, and physical) is a precursor to learning” (p.64). Although the learner has a central role in defining activity, very little, if any, meaningful activity is accomplished individually. Activity theory contends that learning and doing are inseparable, and that they are initiated by intention (Jurdak & Shahin, 2001). Activity always involves an ensemble of artifacts (instruments, signs, procedures, machines, methods, laws, and

forms of work organization) contrary to cognitive psychologist focus on mental representations. Jonassen and Murphy (1999) argue that Activity theory presents a new perspective for analyzing learning processes and outcomes for designing instruction. He adds: “rather than focusing on knowledge states, it focuses on the activities in which people are engaged, the nature of the tools they use in those activities, the social and contextual relationships among the collaborators in those activities, the goals and intentions of those activities, and the objects or outcomes of those activities” (p. 68).

Nativism, Empiricism, and Constructivism Theories of cognitive development are situated under three fundamental views related to the origin of knowledge: The empiricist view, the nativist view, and the constructivist view (Saxe, 1991). Byrnes (2007) argues that all nativists share a belief that knowledge is innate. While the empiricist view favors the position that the environment is the source of knowledge, the nativists advocate the need of knowledge structures to organize and categorize experience (Saxe, 1991). The polar opposite of nativism is Empiricism. In this view, it is believed that individuals possess no a priori knowledge but rather that most knowledge is perceived to be acquired through exposure to the world. Constructivists, on the other hand, adopt the premise that knowledge is not inherent in the human mind nor in the environment, but is rather actively constructed by the individual as a result of his/her interaction with the social and physical environment (Millroy, 1999). As a matter of fact, constructivism derives most of its ideas from Piaget’s theory of cognitive development.

and those interested in “ethnomathematics”, where D’Ambrosio (1985) is a key figure. Both groups of researchers call for a new conceptualization of mathematics that is rooted in nonacademic practices. The work of these groups focuses on three main issues: Analysis of school practice, investigation of the transfer of school knowledge to out-of-school situations, and using the social theory of practice to challenge conventional cognitive theory.

Jerome Bruner’s Representation Theory With the hypothesis that “any subject can be taught effectively in some intellectually honest form to any child at any stage of development” (Bruner, 1960, p.47), Bruner envisioned learning as successively proceeding through three hierarchical stages. He held the view that experience is coded and processed in such a way to ensure its retrieval when needed. Such a coding system Bruner calls representation. He converted his ideas about modes of representation into chronologically structured stages of development and claimed that understanding in any domain must involve three modes of representation: enactive through habits of acting , iconic in the form of pictorial images and symbolic through written symbols like language. “By enactive, I mean a mode of representing past events through appropriate motor response” (Bruner, 1964, p. 69). In this regard, he agrees with Vygotsky’s notion of ZPD in helping children understand and master a concept by proceeding from physical practical actions then using imagery and pictorial representation after which written symbols can be used. Not surprisingly, in his more recent work, Bruner expanded his theoretical framework to encompass the philosophical and sociocultural aspects of learning. In his approach to instruction, Bruner emphasized

four major ideas: structure of the discipline, readiness to learn which depends on rich learning experiences and enthusiastic teachers, intuition, and motivation. He also calls for curriculum to be organized in a spiral manner so that the student continually builds upon what he/she already learned. Perhaps one of the most important implications of Bruner’s work is the significant role that different modes of representation play in learning mathematics. Cramer (2003) argues that experiencing the benefits of using multiple representations as well as allowing possible translations among these different modes of representations helps teachers become more aware of the weaknesses inherent in any curriculum and thus respond by incrementing it with outside resources. In addition to the significant role that external representations play in students’ active construction of mathematical concepts, many studies have investigated the impact of students’ beliefs, feelings and affective representation in enhancing or hindering mathematical understanding in the classroom. Goldin (2003) calls for “a good balance between the standard manipulation of formal notational systems… and the development of other representational modes: imagistic thinking, involving visualization; visual imagery, pattern recognition, and analogical reasoning; heuristic planning, involving diverse problem solving strategies; and affective representation” (p.283). By the same token, Monk (2003) highlights the importance of providing concrete, multiple embodiments that are meaningful to students, and further explains “the goal is not to select one or two representational forms for students to learn and use in all situations but, rather, to teach students to adapt representations to a particular context and purpose and even to use several representations at the same time.

This goal represents a shift from representation to representing” (p.260).

Beyond Theories: Epistemological Frameworks As has been noted by several theorists (Byrnes, 2007, Lesh & Doerr, 2003), perhaps there no longer exist "grand theories" such as Piaget's which attempt to explain many aspects of cognition. Instead we have many "micro-theories" or what Lesh and Doerr (2003) calls “models” designed to account for a “specific purposes in specific situations” (p.526). An increasing number of theorists are developing numerous epistemological frameworks by combining constructs from several theories which may potentially contribute to our understanding of how mathematics is learned. The current trend in mathematics education research is converging towards adopting a models and modeling perspective for the teaching and learning of mathematics (Lesh & Doerr, 2003). The main gist underlying this call is not only to progress an agenda of research that builds on recommendations of constructivism, but also to include “a wealth of recent advances in fields of mathematics ranging from complexity theory to game theory- where a variety of different types of systems thinking tends to be highlighted.” (p.555)

Complexity Science Recent research in complexity science has revolutionized the conduct and method of science by revealing new perspectives and possibilities that challenge beliefs and ideas of contemporary learning theories. The exponential advancement of innovations in digital technologies is pushing the boundaries of learning beyond existing traditional classrooms by creating learning environments that seamlessly

capture and model complexity in the world. Such technologically-supported environments are transforming education and expanding the concept of schooling beyond school settings. In an article entitled, Understanding Learning Systems: Mathematics Education and Complexity Science, Davies and Simmt (2003) describes mathematics classes as “ adaptive and self- organizing complex systems where “learning is understood in terms of ongoing, recursively elaborative adaptations through which systems maintain their coherences within their dynamic circumstances” (p.138). While complexity scientists agree with the views advocated by situated learning theories and social constructivism where the emphasis is placed on analyzing the dynamics of emerging experiences in context, however they transcend individual and social constructivism in calling for a conceptual shift away from mathematics as content and toward “ emergent terms ” (Davies & Sumara, 2007). This perspective is concurrent with the view that mathematics is socially and culturally constructed. Within the realm of complexity science and where the world is seen as becoming increasingly complex, the discrete distinction between teaching mathematics and teaching children collapses in favor of a rather nested, integrated whole whose entities are inseparable. In this context, Davies and Simmt (2003) explain: “To teach children well, we argue, we must conceive of our activity in terms of active participation in the body of mathematics knowledge by creating the conditions for the emergence of bottom-up, locally controlled, collective learning systems” (p. 163). Furthermore, the authors emphasize five basic conditions that help facilitate the creation of a classroom community: a) internal diversity which, calls for respecting

that before the pendulum of reform sweeps too far to the right, it may be time to consider alternative, but not necessarily competing, perspectives on the development of mathematical knowledge” (p. 436). With so many voices contributing recommendations and standards, teachers are faced with overwhelming challenges in this STEM world we live in. Interestingly enough, while so many considerations are directed toward “what” mathematics students should learn, only a dim initiative has been advanced on “how” children should learn. The impact that theories of learning can have for the teacher in the mathematics classroom is far more complex than what it seems to be. Lamon (2003) argues that the powerful recommendations set by Piaget’s cognitive constructivist view, as well as Vygotsky’s sociocultural perspective of acculturation and negotiation can be less effective if misinterpreted and overgeneralized in the mathematics classroom. Regardless of their graduate training or experience, we argue that teachers bring to their practice what might be called “personal theories” of teaching and learning. These theories may be only partly conscious; however these epistemologies are what guide teachers’ everyday decisions about planning, subject content, and classroom behavior. “Personal theories” of teaching and learning grow out of our experiences as students and teachers and begin developing while we are children. We develop predispositions toward certain learning "styles", just as we gravitate toward certain teaching styles. If we are to trace the literature on best practices for improvement of teaching a useful focal point emerge which is an analysis of the degree to which teachers’ practice is consistent with their introspection about teaching-their “personal theory.”

Summary The 21st^ century is an exciting and challenging time for mathematics teachers as the opportunities to expand teaching and learning are becoming more and more pervasive in formal and informal education. Associated with this growth are the increasing number of demands and expectations behooved on teachers to serve as leaders enacting cutting-edge instructional practices in their classrooms. Our world, through the use of complex satellite systems, is connected with an invisible digital network that makes todays’ classrooms inevitably global. Students now learn from a multitude of resources that range from textbooks to live videoconferences with people geographically separated by thousands of miles. The world is becoming more open to students through live, streamed videos enabling them to see the world “as it happens” without any controls. In this climate, teachers are expected to be well- versed in the newest learning technologies and products in order to best prepare students for the global digital workforce. However, in the midst of increased technology, access to resources and to professional development in today’s highly diverse schools remains the fundamental quandary dodging opportunities for improvement in teaching and learning (Smaldino, Lowther, Russell, 2012). While teachers are called upon to expand their professional knowledge and growth by staying informed of new technologies that have positive impact on students learning, very little effort is advanced to increase teacher capability to use assistive technologies to facilitate student success. With heightened tension from policy makers, the public as well as educational media, teachers find themselves pressured to deliver quality math instruction to ensure

that all students achieve academic proficiency in a STEM world (Chahine & King, 2012). On February 17, 2009, President Obama signed the American Recovery and Reinvestment Act (ARRA), spurring the Race to the Top (RTT) initiative, which was a way to invest in the nation's education system in an effort to reform schools. President Obama declared: “America will not succeed in the 21st century unless we do a far better job of educating our sons and daughters…The race starts today” (Bosser, 2012, p.1). RTT focuses on more rigorous standards and closer evaluation of teachers whose performance is inextricably linked to students’ achievement. However, to empower teachers toward better teaching performance and thereby increase student academic progress, perhaps we need to provide more incentives for teachers to undertake the challenge of educating future generations. In this critical educational climate, it’s reasonable to argue that what mathematics teachers need more than ever, is sincere support and collaboration that is free from any political obligation. Instead of focusing on issues of accountability and fueling efforts towards more exam–driven instruction, perhaps teachers need more support to understand how students think, to uncover cultural and social backgrounds that help interpret their ways of thinking, and to capitalize on those practices that maintain equity, diversity and high quality instruction for all students so that more student are seeing success with math and liking it too.

References

Bosser, U. (2012). Race to the Top: What Have We Learned from the States So Far? A State-by-State Evaluation of Race to the Top Performance. Center

for American Progress. Retrieved from http://www.americanprogress.org/wp -content/ uploads/ issues/ 2012/03/pdf/rtt_states.pdf Brooks, J.C., & Brooks, M. (1993). In search of understanding: The case for constructivist classrooms. Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision & Curriculum. Bruner, J. (1960). Readiness for learning. In J. Bruner (Ed.) In search of a pedagogy: The selected works of Jerome S. Bruner, (2006) Vol. 1 (pp.47-56). London & New York: Routledge. Bruner, J. (1964). The course of cognitive growth. In J. Bruner (Ed.) In search of a pedagogy: The selected works of Jerome S. Bruner, (2006) Vol. 1 (pp.68-89). London & New York: Routledge. Byrnes, J. (2007). Cognitive development and learning in instructional contexts. Boston, MA: Pearson. Carlson, N.R. (2010). Psychology: the science of behavior. Toronto, Canada: Pearson Canada Inc. Chahine, I.C. (in press). The impact of using multiple modalities on students' acquisition of fractional knowledge: An international study in embodied mathematics across semiotic cultures. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior. Chahine, I.C., King, H. (2012). Investigating Lebanese teachers’ mathematical, pedagogical and self-efficacy profiles: A case study. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.5339/nmejre.

Confrey, J. & Stohl, V. (2004). On evaluating curricular effectiveness: Judging the quality of k-

students’ motivation. The Clearing House , 75 , 132–135. Cornell, S., Hartmann,D. ( 2007). Ethnicity and race: Making identities in a changing world. Thousand Oaks, CA: Pine Forge Press. Hidi, S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000). Motivating the academically unmotivated: A critical issue for the 21st century. Review of Educational Research , 70 , 151–179. Hoffman, B., & Spatariu, A. (2008). The influence of self-efficacy and metacognitive prompting on math problem solving efficiency. Contemporary Educational Psychology , 33 (4), 875-893. Illeris, K. (2004). The three dimensions of learning. Malabar, Fl.: Krieger Pub. Co. Jonassen, D., & Murphy,L.(1999). Activity theory as a framework for designing constructivist learning environments. Educational Technology of Research and Development , 47 (1), 61-79. Jitendra, A. (2002). Teaching students math problem-solving through graphic representations. Teaching Exceptional Children , 34 (4), 34-38. Jurdak, M. and Shahin, I. (2001). Problem solving activity in the workplace and the school: The case of constructing solids. Educational Studies in Mathematics Education, 47 , 297-

Kincheloe, J.L. (2004). Critical pedagogy. New York, NY: Peter Lang Publishing Inc. Lamon, S. (2003). Beyond Constructivism: An improved Fitness Metaphor for the acquisition of Mathematical Knowledge. In R. Lesh & H. Doerr (Eds.), Beyond constructivism: Models and modeling perspectives on mathematics problem solving,

learning and teaching (pp.435-447). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. Lave, J., Smith, S., & Butler, M. (1990). Problem solving as an everyday practice. In R. Charles, & E. Silver (Eds.), The teaching and assessing of mathematical problem solving (pp. 61 - 81). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Leont’ev, A. N. (1978). Activity, consciousness, personality. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Lesh, R., & Doerr, H. (2003). In what ways does models and modeling perspective move beyond constructivism? In R.Lesh & H. Doerr (Eds), Beyond constructivism: Models and modeling perspectives on mathematics problem solving, learning and teaching (pp.519-556). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Millroy, W.L. (1992). An ethnographic study of the mathematical ideas of a group of carpenters. Journal for Research in Math Education Monographs , 5 (0883-9530). Monk, S. (2003). Representation in school mathematics: Learning to graph and graphing to learn. In J. Kilpatrick, W.G. Martin, & D. Schifter (Eds.), A research companion to Principles and Standards for school mathematics , (pp. 250-261). Reston, VA: The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics Inc. National Commission on Excellence in Education. (1983). A nation at risk: The imperative of educational reform. Washington, Dc: Author. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (2000). Principles and

Standards for School Mathematics. Retrieved from http://www.nctm.org/standards/conte nt.asp National Governors Association Center for

Best Practices (NGA Center) and the Council of Chief State School Officers (CCSSO) (2010). Common core state standards initiative. Washington, DC. Authors. The Common Core State Standards from http://www.corestandards.org.

Nunes, T., Schliemann, A.D., and Carraher, D.W. (1993). Street mathematics and school mathematics. Boston, MA: Cambridge University Press. Newell, A., & Simon, H.A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, N.J: Prentice Hall Incorporation. Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York, NY: International University Press. Piaget, J. (1977). The development of thought: Equilibration of cognitive structures. New York, NY: The Viking Press. Post, T. (1988). Some notes on the nature of mathematics. In T. Post (Ed.), Teaching mathematics in Grades K- 8: Research-based methods , (pp. 1- 19). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Pritchard, A. (2005). Ways of learning: Learning theories and learning styles in the classroom. Abingdon, VA: David Fulton Publishers. Putnam, R.T., Lampert, M., & Peterson, P.L. (1989). Alternative perspectives on knowing mathematics in elementary schools. Review of Research in Education, 16 , 57-149. Rittle-Johnson, B., Siegler, R.S. & Alibali, M.W. (2001). Developing conceptual understanding and procedural skill in mathematics: An

iterative process. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93 , 346-

Saxe, G.B. (1991). Culture and cognitive development: Studies in mathematical understanding. Hillsdale, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Association. Schoenfeld, A. (1983). Beyond the purely cognitive: Belief systems, social cognitions, and metacognitions as driving forces in intellectual performance. In T. Carpenter, J. Dossey, & J. Koehler (Eds.), Classics in mathematics education research (2004), (pp.110- 133).Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics Inc. Schmidt, W., Houang, R., Cogan, L. (2011). Preparing future mathematics teachers. Science , 332 , 1266-1267. Scribner, S. (1986). Thinking in action: Some characteristics of practical thought. In R.J. Sternberg & R.k.Wagner (Eds.), Practical intelligence nature and origins of competence in the everyday world (pp. 13- 30). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Thompson, B., & Thornton, H. (2002). The transition from extrinsic to intrinsic motivation in the classroom: A first year experience. Education, 122 , 785 – 792. Tuckman, B. W. (1992). Educational psychology: From theory to application. Orlando, Fl.: Harcourt Brace Jovanvich. Turner, J. C. (1982). Toward a cognitive redefinition of the social group. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Social identity and intergroup behavior (pp. 15-40). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.