Docsity
Docsity

Prepare for your exams
Prepare for your exams

Study with the several resources on Docsity


Earn points to download
Earn points to download

Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan


Guidelines and tips
Guidelines and tips

Foolproof A Sampling of Mathematical Folk Humor | MAT 080, Study notes of Algebra

Material Type: Notes; Class: Intermediate Algebra; Subject: Mathematics; University: Sauk Valley Community College; Term: Unknown 1989;

Typology: Study notes

Pre 2010

Uploaded on 08/05/2009

koofers-user-2ri
koofers-user-2ri 🇺🇸

10 documents

1 / 11

Toggle sidebar

This page cannot be seen from the preview

Don't miss anything!

bg1
Foolproof: A Sampling
of Mathematical Folk
Humor
Paul Renteln and Alan Dundes
24 NOTICES OF THE AMS VOLUME 52, NUMBER 1
In the discipline known as folkloristics [D1] (the
study of folklore), a folk is defined as any
group whatsoever that shares at least one
common linking factor. The factor could be na-
tionality, ethnicity, religion, or occupation.
Members of a profession would also qualify as a
folk group. Hence, mathematicians constitute a
folk. And, like all folk groups, mathematicians have
their own folk speech (slang), proverbs, limericks,
and jokes, among other forms of folklore. It is pre-
cisely the folklore of a group that defines that
group. So, mathematicians as a group share a com-
mon core of mathematical folklore. Some of this
folklore tends to be quite esoteric and intelligible
only to members of the group. Outsiders not pos-
sessing the requisite mathematical vocabulary and
knowledge rarely know such esoteric material, and
even if they did they would probably not under-
stand it. But there is also exoteric mathematical
folklore that is known to a limited number of out-
siders, for example, physicists, chemists, and en-
gineers. Much of this exoteric folklore consists of
classic jokes contrasting members of different but
related academic disciplines. We intend to offer a
brief sampling of both esoteric and exoteric math-
ematical folklore, concentrating on humorous gen-
res such as jokes. We are persuaded that these
data not only serve as a basis for identity among
mathematicians but also provide a unique window
on mathematical culture in general, and even a
clue as to the nature of mathematical thinking.
As the readership of the Notices is principally
professional mathematicians, we do not feel it is
necessary to explain all the data we present. Most
readers will already be familiar with many if not
all of the examples given here. That is to be ex-
pected. All folklore exists in multiple forms and
demonstrates variation. There are often many ver-
sions of a classic joke or legend. The same absent-
minded professor joke, for example, may be at-
tached to various historical individuals, for
example, Norbert Wiener and John von Neumann.
So it is likely that each reader will recognize a par-
ticular item even though he or she might have
heard it (orally) in a slightly different form.1
Making Fun of Mathematics
Many English-language mathematical jokes are
based on word play involving standard mathe-
matical concepts and terminology. In fact, many of
the jokes involve food items, which may be a re-
flection of the fact that some mathematical con-
cepts are hard to digest, or difficult to swallow:
Q: What’s purple and commutes?
A: An abelian grape.
Paul Renteln is professor and chair of the Department of
Physics at California State University, San Bernardino,
and visiting associate in the Department of Mathematics
at the California Institute of Technology. His email address
is prenteln@csusb.edu.
Alan Dundes is professor of anthropology and folklore at
the University of California, Berkeley. His email address
is carolynb@sscl.berkeley.edu.
1Many of the following texts come from oral tradition,
but we have also borrowed freely from various Internet
websites, including
http://www.xs4all.nl/~jcdverha/scijokes;
http://www.math.utah.edu/~cherk/mathjokes.html;
http://www.workjoke.com/projoke22.htm;
http://users.ox.ac.uk/~invar/links/jokes.html;
and
http://outreach.math.wisc.edu/local/
miscellany/MathJokes.htm.
pf3
pf4
pf5
pf8
pf9
pfa

Partial preview of the text

Download Foolproof A Sampling of Mathematical Folk Humor | MAT 080 and more Study notes Algebra in PDF only on Docsity!

Foolproof: A Sampling

of Mathematical Folk

Humor

Paul Renteln and Alan Dundes

24 NOTICES OF THE AMS VOLUME 52, NUMBER 1

I

n the discipline known as folkloristics [D1] (the study of folklore), a folk is defined as any group whatsoever that shares at least one common linking factor. The factor could be na- tionality, ethnicity, religion, or occupation. Members of a profession would also qualify as a folk group. Hence, mathematicians constitute a folk. And, like all folk groups, mathematicians have their own folk speech (slang), proverbs, limericks, and jokes, among other forms of folklore. It is pre- cisely the folklore of a group that defines that group. So, mathematicians as a group share a com- mon core of mathematical folklore. Some of this folklore tends to be quite esoteric and intelligible only to members of the group. Outsiders not pos- sessing the requisite mathematical vocabulary and knowledge rarely know such esoteric material, and even if they did they would probably not under- stand it. But there is also exoteric mathematical folklore that is known to a limited number of out- siders, for example, physicists, chemists, and en- gineers. Much of this exoteric folklore consists of classic jokes contrasting members of different but related academic disciplines. We intend to offer a brief sampling of both esoteric and exoteric math- ematical folklore, concentrating on humorous gen- res such as jokes. We are persuaded that these data not only serve as a basis for identity among mathematicians but also provide a unique window

on mathematical culture in general, and even a clue as to the nature of mathematical thinking. As the readership of the Notices is principally professional mathematicians, we do not feel it is necessary to explain all the data we present. Most readers will already be familiar with many if not all of the examples given here. That is to be ex- pected. All folklore exists in multiple forms and demonstrates variation. There are often many ver- sions of a classic joke or legend. The same absent- minded professor joke, for example, may be at- tached to various historical individuals, for example, Norbert Wiener and John von Neumann. So it is likely that each reader will recognize a par- ticular item even though he or she might have heard it (orally) in a slightly different form. 1

Making Fun of Mathematics

Many English-language mathematical jokes are based on word play involving standard mathe- matical concepts and terminology. In fact, many of the jokes involve food items, which may be a re- flection of the fact that some mathematical con- cepts are hard to digest, or difficult to swallow:

Q: What’s purple and commutes? A: An abelian grape.

Paul Renteln is professor and chair of the Department of Physics at California State University, San Bernardino, and visiting associate in the Department of Mathematics at the California Institute of Technology. His email address is prenteln@csusb.edu. Alan Dundes is professor of anthropology and folklore at the University of California, Berkeley. His email address is carolynb@sscl.berkeley.edu.

(^1) Many of the following texts come from oral tradition, but we have also borrowed freely from various Internet websites, including h t t p : / / w w w. x s 4 a l l. n l / ~ j c d v e r h a / s c i j o k e s ; http://www.math.utah.edu/~cherk/mathjokes.html; h t t p : / / w w w. w o r k j o k e. c o m / p r o j o k e 2 2. h t m ; http://users.ox.ac.uk/~invar/links/jokes.html; and h t t p : / / o u t r e a c h. m a t h. w i s c. e d u / l o c a l / miscellany/MathJokes.htm.

JANUARY 2005 NOTICES OF THE AMS 25

There are several other variants of this joke:

Q: What’s purple, commutes, and is worshipped by a limited number of people? A: A finitely-venerated abelian grape.

Q: What is lavender and commutes? A: An abelian semigrape.

Q: What is purple and all of its offspring have been committed to institutions? A: A simple grape: it has no normal subgrapes.

Q: What’s purple, round, and doesn’t get much for Christmas? A: A finitely presented grape.

Q: What’s an abelian group under addition, closed, associative, distributive, and bears a curse? A: The Ring of the Nibelung.

Q: What’s nutritious and commutes? A: An abelian soup.

Q: What’s hot, chunky, and acts on a polygon? A: Dihedral soup.

Q: What’s sour, yellow, and equivalent to the Axiom of Choice? A: Zorn’s Lemon.

There are thousands of lemmas in mathemat- ics, so in principle the same word play would work on any one of them, yet this joke based on Zorn’s lemma continues to be popular. Why should this be so? The reason for this becomes clearer when we recall that the Axiom of Choice can be used to prove many counterintuitive results in set theory, such as the Banach-Tarski paradox. This leads some mathematicians to reject the Axiom of Choice. Yet because it is so useful (and because it seems so innocuous at first glance), many mathemati- cians accept it, but with strong reservations. They find it distasteful, or unpalatable, or, if you will, a bitter fruit. Here is another one, even more revealing:

Q: What is brown, furry, runs to the sea, and is equiv- alent to the Axiom of Choice? A: Zorn’s lemming.

Lemmings are known to follow each other blindly into the sea, where they drown. The impli- cation of this version seems to be that mathe- maticians who rely upon the Axiom of Choice are all lemmings who blindly follow one another and who may all be headed for intellectual death.

Q: What is green and homeomorphic to the open unit interval? A: The real lime.

Q: What’s yellow, linear, normed, and complete? A: A Bananach space.

Q: What do you call a young eigensheep? A: A lamb, duh!

Q: What’s the value of a contour integral around Western Europe? A: Zero, because all the Poles are in Eastern Europe. Addendum: Actually, there ARE some Poles in West- ern Europe, but they are removable! 2

Q: Why did the mathematician name his dog “Cauchy”? A: Because he left a residue at every pole.

Q: What is a topologist? A: Someone who cannot distinguish between a doughnut and a coffee cup.

Q: Why didn’t Newton discover group theory? A: Because he wasn’t Abel.

Q: What do you get if you cross an elephant and a banana? A: |elephant||banana|sin(theta).

(^2) There are several shaggy dog stories based on wordplay in complex analysis. For example: A bunch of Polish scientists decided to flee their re- pressive government by hijacking an airliner and forcing the pilot to fly them to a western country. They drove to the airport, forced their way on board a large passenger jet, and found there was no pilot on board. Terrified, they listened as the sirens got louder. Finally, one of the sci- entists suggested that since he was an experimentalist, he would try to fly the aircraft. He sat down at the con- trols and tried to figure them out. The sirens got louder and louder. Armed men surrounded the jet. The would- be pilot’s friends cried out, “Please, please take off now!!! Hurry!!!” The experimentalist calmly replied, “Have pa- tience. I’m just a simple pole in a complex plane.” A group of Polish tourists is flying on a small airplane through the Grand Canyon on a sightseeing tour. The tour guide announces: “On the right of the airplane, you can see the famous Bright Angle Falls.” The tourists leap out of their seats and crowd to the windows on the right side. This causes a dynamic imbalance, and the plane violently rolls to the side and crashes into the canyon wall. All aboard are lost. The moral of this episode is: always keep your poles off the right side of the plane. For a classification of shaggy dog stories, including ones with punch lines based on “an Axiom of Science”, see [B].

JANUARY 2005 NOTICES OF THE AMS 27

Q: What does a topologist call a virgin? A: Simply connected.^7

Some joking questions are adaptations of stan- dard folkloristic forms. The question “How many xxxx’s does it take to change a lightbulb” has sev- eral answers [D2], including the following:

Q: How many topologists does it take to change a lightbulb? A: Just one, but what will you do with the dough- nut?

Q: How many number theorists does it take to change a lightbulb? A: This is not known, but it is conjectured to be an elegant prime.

Q: How many geometers does it take to screw in a lightbulb? A: None. You can’t do it with a straightedge and com- pass.

Q: How many analysts does it take to screw in a light- bulb? A: Three. One to prove existence, one to prove uniqueness, and one to derive a nonconstructive algorithm to do it.

Q: How many Bourbakists does it take to replace a lightbulb? A: Changing a lightbulb is a special case of a more general theorem concerning the maintenance and repair of an electrical system. To establish upper and lower bounds for the number of personnel required, we must determine whether the sufficient conditions of Lemma 2.1 (Availability of personnel) and those of Corollary 2.3.55 (Motivation of personnel) apply. If and only if these conditions are met, we derive the result by an application of the theorems in Section 3.1123. The resulting upper bound is, of course, a result in an abstract measure space, in the weak-* topology.

Q: How many mathematicians does it take to screw in a lightbulb? A: 0.999999….

In addition to making fun of beginning mathe- matics students who have difficulty grasping the equality 0_._ 9999 · · · = 1 , this joke also shows how mathematics is essentially a solitary endeavor, as it only takes one mathematician to do it. This can be contrasted with many of the other standard an- swers to this question, which demonstrate that groups of people are often required. In fact, the

following text shows that mathematicians are so clever that they can imagine a lightbulb that changes itself!

Q: How many lightbulbs does it take to change a lightbulb? A: One, if it knows its own Gödel number.

This joke also expresses the fear that mathe- matics itself may be inconsistent (and perhaps even lead to paradoxes), as Gödel numbers were used by Gödel to prove his two celebrated incom- pleteness theorems, both of which shook the foun- dations of mathematics. The same anxiety sur- faces in the following parody of the traditional joke: “Why did the chicken cross the road? To get to the other side.”

Q: Why did the chicken cross the road? A: Gödel: It cannot be proved whether the chicken crossed the road.

Some other answers to this question provided by famous mathematicians include:

Q: Why did the chicken cross the road? A: Erdös: It was forced to do so by the chicken-hole principle.

Q: Why did the chicken cross the road? A: Riemann: The answer appears in Dirichlet’s lec- tures.

Q: Why did the chicken cross the road? A: Fermat: It did not fit on the margin on this side.

A good mathematician always looks for possi- ble counterexamples to unproved conjectures. Here is an amusing variant of the chicken crossing joke, demonstrating that the notion of “other side” is not always as straightforward as it seems:

Q: Why did the chicken cross the Möbius strip? A: To get to the other–er….

If you cannot prove the theorem, prove a dif- ferent one:

Q: Why did the chicken cross the Möbius strip? A: To get to the same side.

Although puns and one-liners occur orally, mod- ern folklore is transmitted by photocopier, email, and the Internet, frequently in the form of lists [DP]. An example of this form is the following “Top Ten Excuses for Not Doing Homework”:

  • I accidentally divided by zero and my paper burst into flames.

(^7) Here, again, the jokester reveals a lack of understand-

ing of either mathematics or anatomy.

28 NOTICES OF THE AMS VOLUME 52, NUMBER 1

  • Isaac Newton’s birthday.
  • I could only get arbitrarily close to my textbook. I couldn’t actually reach it.
  • I have the proof, but there isn’t room to write it in this margin.
  • I was watching the World Series and got tied up trying to prove that it converged.
  • I have a solar-powered calculator and it was cloudy.
  • I locked the paper in my trunk, but a four- dimensional dog got in and ate it.
  • I couldn’t figure out whether i am the square of negative one or i is the square root of negative one.
  • I took time out to snack on a doughnut and a cup of coffee [and] I spent the rest of the night try- ing to figure which one to dunk.
  • I could have sworn I put the home work inside a Klein bottle, but this morning I couldn’t find it.

Every mathematician relies on the truth of prior propositions proved by other mathematicians, and it is ordinarily impossible for a person to check the validity of every proof upon which his work is based. For this reason, mathematicians place tremendous emphasis on the correctness of mathematical proofs. The following list of proof techniques hints at the anxieties felt by many mathematicians regarding the degree to which mathematical truth is dependent upon the trust- worthiness of previous results. This anxiety is ex- acerbated by the fact that some mathematicians have a less rigorous proof style than other math- ematicians. How to prove it. Guide for lecturers.

Proof by vigorous handwaving: Works well in a classroom or seminar setting.

Proof by forward reference: Reference is usually to a forthcoming paper of the author, which is often not as forthcoming as at first.

Proof by funding: How could three different government agencies be wrong?

Proof by example: The author gives only the case n = 2 and suggests that it contains most of the ideas of the general proof.

Proof by omission: “The reader may easily supply the details.” “The other 253 cases are analogous.”

Proof by deferral: “We’ll prove this later in the course.”

Proof by picture: A more convincing form of proof by example. Com- bines well with proof by omission.

Proof by intimidation: “Trivial.”

Proof by seduction: “Convince yourself that this is true!”

Proof by cumbersome notation: Best done with access to at least four alphabets and special symbols.

Proof by exhaustion: An issue or two of a journal devoted to your proof is useful.

Proof by obfuscation: A long plotless sequence of true and/or meaning- less syntactically related statements.

Proof by wishful citation: The author cites the negation, converse, or gener- alization of a theorem from the literature to sup- port his claims.

Proof by eminent authority: “I saw Karp in the elevator and he said it was prob- ably NP-complete.”

Proof by personal communication: “Eight-dimensional colored cycle stripping is NP- complete [Karp, personal communication].”

Proof by reduction to the wrong problem: “To see that infinite-dimensional colored cycle stripping is decidable, we reduce it to the halting problem.”

Proof by reference to inaccessible literature: The author cites a simple corollary of a theorem to be found in a privately circulated memoir of the Slovenian Philological Society, 1883.

Proof by importance: A large body of useful consequences all follow from the proposition in question.

Proof by accumulated evidence: Long and diligent search has not revealed a coun- terexample.

Proof by cosmology: The negation of the proposition is unimaginable or meaningless. Popular for proofs of the existence of God.

30 NOTICES OF THE AMS VOLUME 52, NUMBER 1

N bottles of beer on the wall, N bottles of beer, You take one down, and pass it around, N − 1 bottles of beer on the wall.

The song begins at N = 100 and then repeats until there are no bottles of beer remaining. It is often sung by children on long car rides, as a way of keep- ing them busy. The following mathematical variant is an amusing way to keep the children occupied a bit longer:

Aleph-null bottles of beer on the wall, Aleph-null bottles of beer, You take one down, and pass it around, Aleph-null bottles of beer on the wall.

Making Fun of Mathematicians

Most of the above texts would not be likely to cir- culate among nonmathematicians, as they rely on specialized knowledge of the field. However, there is a substantial body of humor involving mathe- maticians themselves, typically contrasted with other scientists, which is more accessible to a gen- eral audience [G]. These jokes are often told by physicists and engineers, but they are also related by mathematicians, despite the fact that they are typically the object of ridicule. One reason for this may be that the mathematician is more concerned with ideas than with reality:

Engineers think that equations approx- imate the real world. Physicists think that the real world approximates equa- tions. Mathematicians are unable to make the connection.

Another variant of this reads:

An engineer thinks that his equations are an approximation to reality. A physicist thinks reality is an approximation to his equations. A mathematician doesn’t care.

The stereotypical mathematician is depicted as being impractical, abstract, and not concerned about everyday affairs:

An engineer, a physicist, and a mathe- matician are staying in a hotel. The en- gineer wakes up and smells smoke. He goes out into the hallway and sees a fire, so he fills a trash can from his room with water and douses the fire. He goes back to bed. Later, the physicist wakes up and smells the smoke. He opens his door and sees a fire in the hallway. He walks down the hall to a fire hose and after calculating the flame velocity, dis- tance, water pressure, trajectory, and

so forth, extinguishes the fire with the minimum amount of water and energy needed. Later the mathematician wakes up and smells smoke. He goes to the hall, sees the fire and then the fire hose. He thinks for a moment and then exclaims, “Ah, a solution exists!” and then goes back to bed.

Here is another version:

A physicist and a mathematician are sitting in a faculty lounge. Suddenly, the coffee machine catches on fire. The physicist grabs a bucket and leaps to- ward the sink, fills the bucket with water, and puts out the fire. Second day, the same two sit in the same lounge. Again the coffee machine catches on fire. This time, the mathematician stands up, gets a bucket, and hands the bucket to the physicist, thus reducing the problem to a previously solved one.

The principle of reducing a theorem to a previ- ously proved theorem is often misinterpreted by nonmathematicians as that of reducing a “problem” to a previously solved one. The result is some- times a bit unexpected:

A mathematician and an engineer are on a desert island. They find two palm trees with one coconut each. The engi- neer shinnies up one tree, gets the co- conut, and eats it. The mathematician shinnies up the other tree, gets the co- conut, climbs the other tree and puts it there. “Now we’ve reduced it to a prob- lem we know how to solve.”

In the following story the mathematician is so wrapped up in his own mathematical world that he appears heartless:

There are a mathematician and a physi- cist and a burning building with people inside. There are a fire hydrant and a hose on the sidewalk. The physicist has to put the fire out…so, he attaches the hose to the hydrant, puts the fire out, and saves the house and the family. Then they put the people back in the house, set it on fire, and ask the mathematician to solve the problem. So, he takes the hose off the hydrant and lays it on the sidewalk. “Now I’ve reduced it to a previously solved problem,” and walks away.

The mathematical solution to a problem is often the most elegant but does not solve the original problem in an effective manner:

JANUARY 2005 NOTICES OF THE AMS 31

One day a farmer called up an engi- neer, a physicist, and a mathematician and asked them to fence in the largest possible area with the least amount of fence. The engineer made the fence in a circle and proclaimed that he had the most efficient design. The physicist made a long, straight line and proclaimed “We can assume the length is infinite…” and pointed out that fencing off half of the Earth was certainly a more efficient way to do it. The mathematician just laughed at them. He built a tiny fence around himself and said, “I declare myself to be on the outside.”

Some mathematical constructions, when applied to the real world, give rather absurd results:

A biologist, a physicist, and a mathe- matician were sitting in a street café watching the crowd. Across the street they saw a man and a woman entering a building. Ten minutes later they reap- peared together with a third person. “They have multiplied,” said the biologist. “Oh no, an error in measurement,” the physicist sighed. “If exactly one person enters the building now, it will be empty again,” the mathematician concluded.

Three men with degrees in mathematics, physics, and biology are locked up in dark rooms for research reasons. A week later the researchers open the door and the bi- ologist steps out and reports: “Well, I sat around until I started to get bored, then I searched the room and found a tin which I smashed on the floor. There was food in it which I ate when I got hungry. That’s it.” Then they free the man with the de- gree in physics and he says: “I walked along the walls to get an image of the room’s geometry, then I searched it. There was a metal cylinder at five feet into the room and two feet left of the door. It felt like a tin and I threw it at the left wall at the right angle and velocity for it to crack open.” Finally, the researchers open the third door and hear a faint voice out of the darkness: “Let C be an open can.”

Here is a variant with a more dire consequence:

There was a mad scientist (a mad … social…scientist) who kidnapped three colleagues, an engineer, a physicist, and a mathematician, and locked each of them in separate cells with plenty of canned food and water but no can opener. A month later, returning, the

mad scientist went to the engineer’s cell and found it long empty. The engineer had constructed a can opener from pocket trash, used aluminum shavings and dried sugar to make an explosive, and escaped. The physicist had worked out the angle necessary to knock the lids off the tin cans by throwing them against the wall. She was developing a good pitching arm and a new quantum the- ory. The mathematician had stacked the unopened cans into a surprising solution to the kissing problem; his desiccated corpse was propped calmly against a wall, and this was inscribed on the floor in blood: Theorem: If I can’t open these cans, I’ll die. Proof: Assume the oppo- site.…

In the following story the mathematician ap- plies flawless logic, but shows an unwillingness to generalize from a single case, a procedure that holds many mathematical pitfalls, but which is often valid in the real world:

A mathematician, a physicist, and an engineer were traveling through Scot- land when they saw a black sheep through the window of the train. “Aha,” says the engineer, “I see that Scottish sheep are black.” “Hmm,” says the physi- cist, “You mean that some Scottish sheep are black.” “No,” says the mathemati- cian, “All we know is that there is at least one sheep in Scotland, and that at least one side of that one sheep is black!”

Here, the mathematician again gives impracti- cal answers to real-world questions:

Three men are in a hot-air balloon. Soon, they find themselves lost in a canyon somewhere. One of the three men says, “I’ve got an idea. We can call for help in this canyon and the echo will carry our voices far.” So he leans over the basket and yells out, “Helloooooo! Where are we?” (They hear the echo several times.) Fifteen minutes later, they hear this echo- ing voice: “Hellooooo! You’re lost!!” One of the men says, “That must have been a mathematician.” Puzzled, one of the other men asks, “Why do you say that?” The reply: “For three reasons: (1) He took a long time to answer, (2) he was absolutely correct, and (3) his answer was absolutely useless.”

Although the three points made in the man’s reply do apply to many mathematicians, there is more that is unspoken. After all, there is something

JANUARY 2005 NOTICES OF THE AMS 33

to devote all of one’s mental energies to working out a solution. It is worth remarking that the corpus of math- ematical folk humor is not closed and that there are constant additions, often reflecting new trends and topical current events. Feminism may be partly responsible for the following traditional joke that critiques the stereotyped notion that women are unable to understand advanced mathematics (and illustrates the stereotype of the arrogant mathe- matician):^8

Two mathematicians are in a bar. The first one says to the second that the av- erage person knows very little about basic mathematics. The second one dis- agrees and claims that most people can cope with a reasonable amount of math. The first mathematician goes off to the washroom, and in his absence the sec- ond calls over the waitress. He tells her that in a few minutes, after his friend has returned, he will call her over and ask her a question. All she has to do is an- swer “one third x cubed.” She repeats “one thir–dex cue?” He repeats “one third x cubed.” She asks, “one thir dex cuebd?” “Yes, that’s right,” he says. So she agrees, and goes off mumbling to herself, “one thir dex cuebd…”. The first guy returns and the second proposes a bet to prove his point, that most people do know something about basic math. He says he will ask the blonde waitress an inte- gral, and the first laughingly agrees. The second man calls over the waitress and asks “what is the integral of x squared?” The waitress says “one third x cubed” and while walking away, turns back and says over her shoulder, “plus a constant!”

The following story illustrates the influence of contemporary events on mathematical humor. As with many jokes, it reflects the fears and anxieties of the general public. Indeed, the joke goes so far as to equate mathematics teachers with terrorists (recall the Unabomber, Theodore Kaczynski), which perhaps explains why mathematics often fails to get the support it deserves!

At New York’s Kennedy Airport today, an individual was arrested trying to board a flight while in possession of a ruler, a protractor, a setsquare, a slide rule, and

a calculator. At a morning press con- ference, Attorney General John Ashcroft said he believes the man is a member of the notorious al-gebra movement.^9 He is being charged by the FBI with carrying weapons of math instruction.

“Al-gebra is a fearsome cult,” Ashcroft said. “They desire average solutions by means and extremes, and sometimes go off on tangents in a search of absolute value. They use secret code names like ‘x’ and ‘y’ and refer to themselves as ‘unknowns’, but we have determined they belong to a common denominator of the axis with coordinates in every country. As the Greek philanderer Isosce- les used to say, there are three sides to every triangle,” Ashcroft declared.

When asked to comment on the arrest, President Bush said, “If God had wanted us to have better weapons of math in- struction, He would have given us more fingers and toes. I am gratified that our government has given us a sine that it is intent on protracting us from those who are willing to disintegrate us with calculus disregard. Under the circum- ferences, we must differentiate their root, make our point, and draw the line.” President Bush warned, “These weapons of math instruction have the potential to decimal everything in their math on a scalene never before seen unless we become exponents of a higher power and begin to factor in random facts of vertex.”

Attorney General Ashcroft said, “Read my ellipse. Their days are numbered as the hypotenuse tightens around their necks.”

What Is Real?

The abundance of joking questions involving plays on mathematical terminology, as well as the jokes involving the stereotype of the mathematician, sug- gest that mathematicians like to play. The delight in playfulness would seem to run counter to the stereotype of the humorless pedant who is con- cerned only with precision, but there is no neces- sary contradiction. A predilection for fantasy and

(^8) It is also interesting that, unlike the traditional blonde jokes, in which the blonde is depicted as being stupid, in this joke the blonde outsmarts the mathematician. For more on the cycle of blonde jokes, see [DP] and the references therein.

(^9) Of course, one irony in this joke is that the word algebra derives from the word al-Jabr that appears in the title of a book written in Baghdad around 825 by the Arab math- ematician Al-Khwarizmi.

34 NOTICES OF THE AMS VOLUME 52, NUMBER 1

whimsy is perhaps related to the ability of the mathematician to invent new mathematical con- cepts. But it also leads to the stereotype of the mathematician as inhabiting his own mathemati- cal universe, to the point where he is unable to cope with everyday problems such as putting out a fire or eating a coconut. In fact, the mathematician is often lost, both literally and metaphorically. As for the disparity between the mathematical world and the real world, one joke text claims that the mathematician is unaware of it, while another claims he is aware of it but does not care. The im- plication is that many mathematicians feel much more at home in the mathematical universe than in the mundane, quotidian, contingent world. The goal of all mathematics is to discover mathemati- cal truth, which does not depend on the here and now. The difficulty, though, is that mathematics cannot appeal to the logic of reality to validate its axiomatic foundation. This is why there is such anx- iety about the consistency of axiomatic systems and the validity of mathematical proofs, as they are the only means of achieving mathematical certainty. Mathematical jokes allude to and confirm and validate the existence of the mathematical uni- verse, and they can only be understood and ap- preciated by the mathematical cognoscenti. The puns reveal what happens when the two worlds col- lide, and pure mathematics becomes tainted or corrupted or trivialized by its encounter. The home- work jokes reveal how difficult it is to inhabit both worlds, as many truths in one world do not trans- late into truths in the other. Yet mathematics is clearly a human endeavor and can solve concrete problems. It is precisely the tension between the mathematical universe and the nonmathematical universe that is central to much of mathematical humor.

Acknowledgements

One of us [PR] would like to thank his wife, Alison Dundes Renteln, for her contributions to this arti- cle and for putting up with his often incompre- hensible jokes.

References [AD] R. A BRAHAMS and A. DUNDES , On elephantasy and ele- phanticide, The Psychoanalytic Review 56 (1969), 225–241. [AH] R. D. A BRAHAMS and J. C. HICKERSON , Cross-fertiliza- tion riddles, Western Folklore 23 (1964), 253–257. [B] J. B RUNVAND , A classification for shaggy dog stories, Journal of American Folkore 76 (1963), 42–68. [D1] A. DUNDES ed., International Folkloristics: Classic Con- tributions by the Founders of Folklore, Rowman and Lit- tlefield Publishers, Lanham, 1999. [D2] ——— , Many hands make light work or caught in the act of screwing in lightbulbs, Western Folklore 40 (1981), 261–266.

[DP] A. DUNDES and C. PAGTER, Why Don’t Sheep Shrink When it Rains?, Syracuse University Press, Syracuse, 2000. [G] C. G ILKEY , The physicist, the mathematician and the engineer: Scientists and the professional slur, Western Folklore 49 (1990), 215–220. [J] B. JACKSON, The greatest mathematician in the world: Norbert Wiener stories, Western Folklore 31 (1972), 1–22. [K] S. KRANTZ, Mathematical anecdotes, The Mathematical Intelligencer 12 (4) (1990), 32–38.