Docsity
Docsity

Prepare for your exams
Prepare for your exams

Study with the several resources on Docsity


Earn points to download
Earn points to download

Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan


Guidelines and tips
Guidelines and tips

Assessing Denver Developmental Screening & Stanford-Binet Tests for Preschool Intelligence, Lecture notes of Pathology

This document evaluates the denver developmental screening test (ddst) and compares its performance with the stanford-binet test in identifying intelligence in a group of 298 preschool children. The study was conducted by john a. Grant and alan m. Gittelsohn and involved administering both tests to the children and analyzing the results using sensitivity, specificity, underreferral, overreferral, and tetrachoric correlation coefficients.

Typology: Lecture notes

2021/2022

Uploaded on 09/12/2022

torley
torley 🇺🇸

4.6

(41)

258 documents

1 / 4

Toggle sidebar

This page cannot be seen from the preview

Don't miss anything!

bg1
Evaluation
of
298
preschool
children
The
Denver
Developmental
Screening
Test
Compared
With
the
Stanford-Binet
Test
JOHN
A.
GRANT,
M.D.,
M.P.H.,
and
ALAN
M.
GITTELSOHN,
Ph.D.
SYSTEMATIC
screening
of
patient
populations
to
facili-
tate
clinical
services
had
its
prac-
tical
beginning
with
the
Kaiser-
Permanente
Clinic
(1)
in
the
early
1950's.
This
approach
had
not
been
seriously
applied
to
pediatric
age
groups,
however,
until
Frankenburg
and
Dodds
(2)
introduced
the
Denver
Develop-
mental
Screening
Test
(DDST)
in
1967.
In
recent
years,
this
test
has
become
widely
used
by
a
Dr.
Grant
is
chief
physician
and
deputy
director,
Community
Pedi-
atric
Center,
University
of
Mary-
land
School
of
Medicine,
and
former
chief,
school
health
sec-
tion,
division
of
maternal
and
child
health,
Maryland
State
De-
partment
of
Health.
Dr.
Gittel-
sohn
is
professor
of
biostatistics,
Johns
Hopkins
University
School
of
Hygiene
and
Public
Health.
Tearsheet
requests
to
John
A.
Grant,
M.D.,
Department
of
Pediatrics,
University
of
Mary-
land
School
of
Medicine
(attn:
Mrs.
Mantik),
Redwood
and
Greene
Sts.,
Baltimore,
Md.
21201.
variety
of
professional
and
non-
professional
workers
in
pediatric
clinics,
at
maternal
and
child
health
conferences,
and
in
chil-
dren
and
youth
projects.
In
their
early
publication,
Frankenburg
and
Dodds
com-
pared
the
performance
of
the
Denver
Developmental
Screen-
ing
Test
in
a
study
population
of
18
children
to
the
Revised
Yale
Developmental
Schedule
(3).
They
found
high
correlations
but
did
not
examine
the
performance
of
the
test
in
more
explicit
ad-
ministrative
terms
of
pathology
discovered
or
pathology
missed.
We
show
how
the
perform-
ance
of
the
Denver
Developmental
Screening
Test
was
evaluated
for
a
specific
population
and
relate
this
kind
of
evaluation
to
the
needs
of
the
service
program
ad-
ministrator.
Method
and
Materials
The
test
was
given
to
590
chil-
dren,
31/2
to
4½/2
years
of
age,
during
an
epidemiologic
study
of
congenital
rubella
(the
parent
study)
in
the
summer
of
1968
(4).
The
children
were
chosen
to
represent
the
population
of
a
semirural
Maryland
county;
they
came
from
a
wide
variety
of
socioeconomic
strata,
as
meas-
ured
by
a
two-index
(education
and
occupation)
Hollingshead
scale
(table
1).
The
primary
goal
of
the
parent
study
was
to
determine
the
prev-
alence
of
congenital
rubella
in
the
sample
population
by
means
of
a
five-part
screening
battery.
The
Denver
Developmental
Screening
Test
was
one
part
of
the
battery.
The
test
was
administered,
under
close
supervision,
by
four
spe-
cially
trained
technicians
and
scored
according
to
the
manual
provided
by
Frankenburg
and
Dodds
in
1968.
Toward
the
end
of
the
parent
study
the
staff
be-
came
concerned
about
the
field
performance
of
the
test
because
of
an
apparently
high
overrefer-
ral
rate,
which
raised
parallel
concern
about
underreferral-a
more
serious
error
in
view
of
the
primary
study
goal.
We
therefore
decided
to
evaluate
the
perform-
ance
of
this
screening
test
in
at-
tempting
to
correctly
identify
the
intelligence
of
a
typical
group
of
May
1972,
Vol.
87,
No.
5
473
pf3
pf4

Partial preview of the text

Download Assessing Denver Developmental Screening & Stanford-Binet Tests for Preschool Intelligence and more Lecture notes Pathology in PDF only on Docsity!

Evaluation of 298 preschool children

The Denver Developmental^ Screening^ Test

Compared With the Stanford-Binet Test

JOHN A. GRANT, M.D., M.P.H., and ALAN M.^ GITTELSOHN,^ Ph.D.

SYSTEMATIC screening of

patient populations^ to^ facili-

tate clinical services^ had^ its^ prac-

tical beginning with^ the^ Kaiser-

Permanente Clinic^ (1)^ in^ the

early 1950's.^ This^ approach^ had

not been^ seriously^ applied^ to

pediatric age^ groups,^ however,

until Frankenburg and Dodds^ (2) introduced the Denver Develop- mental Screening Test^ (DDST) in 1967. In recent years, this^ test has become widely used^ by a

Dr. Grant is^ chief^ physician^ and

deputy director, Community^ Pedi-

atric Center,^ University^ of^ Mary-

land School of Medicine, and

former chief,^ school^ health^ sec-

tion, division of maternal and

child health, Maryland State^ De-

partment of Health. Dr.^ Gittel-

sohn is professor of biostatistics,

Johns Hopkins University School

of Hygiene and Public^ Health.

Tearsheet requests to^ John^ A.

Grant, M.D.,^ Department^ of

Pediatrics, University of^ Mary-

land School^ of^ Medicine^ (attn:

Mrs. Mantik), Redwood^ and

Greene Sts., Baltimore,^ Md.

variety of professional and non-

professional workers in pediatric

clinics, at maternal and child

health conferences, and^ in^ chil-

dren and youth projects. In their early publication, Frankenburg and Dodds com-

pared the performance of^ the

Denver Developmental^ Screen-

ing Test^ in a^ study^ population^ of 18 children to the Revised Yale

Developmental Schedule (3).

They found high correlations but

did not examine the performance

of the test in more explicit ad-

ministrative terms of pathology

discovered or pathology missed.

We show how^ the perform-

ance of the Denver Developmental

Screening Test was evaluated for

a specific population and relate

this kind of evaluation to the

needs of the service program ad-

ministrator.

Method and Materials

The test was given to^ 590 chil-

dren, 31/2^ to^ 4½/2^ years^ of^ age,

during an epidemiologic study of

congenital rubella^ (the^ parent

study) in the summer of 1968

(4). The^ children^ were^ chosen

to represent the population of^ a semirural Maryland county;^ they came from^ a^ wide^ variety^ of socioeconomic strata,^ as^ meas- ured by a two-index (education

and occupation) Hollingshead

scale (table 1).

The primary goal of the parent

study was to determine the^ prev-

alence of congenital rubella^ in^ the

sample population by^ means^ of^ a

five-part screening battery. The

Denver Developmental Screening

Test was one part of the battery.

The test was administered, under

close supervision, by four spe-

cially trained technicians and

scored according to the manual

provided by Frankenburg and

Dodds in 1968. Toward the^ end

of the parent study the^ staff^ be-

came concerned about the field

performance of the test^ because

of an apparently high overrefer-

ral rate, which raised parallel

concern about underreferral-a

more serious error^ in^ view^ of the

primary study goal. We therefore

decided to^ evaluate^ the^ perform-

ance of this screening test^ in^ at-

tempting to correctly identify the

intelligence of^ a^ typical group^ of

May 1972, Vol. 87, No.^5

preschool children^ as^ measured by a standard test.

Planning, funding, and relocat-

ing subjects from the parent study consumed 12 months-longer than anticipated. Samples of 160 DDST "normals," 75 DDST "questionables," and 63 DDST "abnormals" were located and given the Stanford-Binet Test of Intelligence by a team of four ex- perienced (^) psychologists, on leave from the Baltimore City schools,

under the direction of Lewis

Armistead, Ph.D., Maryland State

Department of Health. The dis-

tribution of 298 IQ scores is de-

tailed in table 2. It is slightly

skewed toward the higher score

intervals, reflecting the distribu- tion of socioeconomic status in

the study population.

Since the data needed to calcu-

late the Hollingshead scale (table

1) were not available for either

the county under study or for the

State as a whole, the percentage

distribution of Hollingshead's

original study group is given as

a frame of reference. As would

be expected in^ comparing an

urban industrial community with

a semirural county, a^ shift is seen

in the distribution of the semi-

rural county from class IV to

Table 2. Distribution of 298

actual IQ scores in this study as compared with expected nor- mal distribution

Expected Actual Stanford-Binet percent percent IQ scores distri- distri- bution bution

0-67........... 68-83.......... 84-99.......... 100-115........ 116-131........ 132 +..........

Total..... 100.0 100.

class II, thus giving reassurance that a representative sample was

being studied.

DDST normals, abnormals,^ and

questionables were defined, ac-

cording to Frankenburg^ and

Dodds' manual, as^ follows:

DDST normal: no items failed below the child's age level; at

least one item, dissected by

the age line, passed.

DDST abnormal: at least two

items failed below the child's

age level.

DDST questionable: all others.

A child's total DDST rating

was the lowest rating in any one

of the four sections: gross motor,

fine motor, language, or personal-

Table 1. Range and distribution of^ socioeconomic status^ of^590 study

subjects, as measured by two-index (education and^ occupation)

Hollingshead scale

Percent Score Percent (^) distribution, Social class range, distribution, Hollings- this study this study head's New Haven community 1

I, professional and^ management ......... 11-13^ 3.1^ 2. II, business^ and^ semiprofessional ........ 14-30^ 14.8^ 9. III, clerical^ and^ sales^ .................. 31-49^ 21.9^ 18. IV, skilled workers ................... 50-64 37.8 48. V. unskilled^ workers^ and^ unemployed.... 65-77^ 22.4^ 20. Total .100.0^ 100.

(^1) See text above. SOURCE: Reference 5.

social. Tasks that were refused

were considered questionable.

To analyze the ability of the

Denver Developmental Screening

Test to predict intelligence as

measured by the Stanford-Binet

test, we dichotomized the results

of both reference and screening

tests as follows:

Stanford-Binet Test of Intelli-

gence: Positive =^0 to 83 Negative 84 Denver Developmental Screening Test:

Positive =^ questionable and

abnormal

Negative =^ normal

Table 3 shows the screening

test-reference test two-by-two

contingency table, from which

vlarious evaluative indices may be

calculated (6, 7). The indices of

sensitivity, specificity, underrefer-

ral, overreferral, and^ tetrachoric

r (cosine-pi approximation)

selected for^ this^ study^ were

calculated as follows: Sensitiv-

ity (^) = a+c specificity (^) b (^) +d'

underreferal = (^) c+d'overrefer- c + d ral (^) = (^) a b' and (^) tetrachoric r

+ b

(cosine-pi approximation) =

Section a of table 3 indicates

true positives, section b false

positives, section c false negatives,

and section d true negatives. Sen-

sitivity indicates the ability of a

screening test to detect a true

positive result out of all diseased

persons. Specificity indicates the

ability of^ a^ screening test to^ de-

tect a true negative result out of

all nondiseased persons.

Underreferral is the ratio of

persons screened^ negative^ with

disease to total persons not re-

474 Health Services Reports

tions contribute little or nothing

to correlation of the test with the

Stanford-Binet IQ test. The fine

motor and^ language sections in-

dividually show higher correla-

tion, but a combination of these

two sections is superior to the

test as a whole. This result is not

surprising because the Stanford-

Binet test relies heavily on fine motor and language functions.

Two important limitations of

this (^) evaluation should be noted:

(a) the screening and the refer-

ence tests were (^) separated by a

period of 12 months and (b)

some DDST tasks (for example,

gross motor and personal-social) were not designed to be predic-

tors of intelligence quotients, as

measured by the Stanford-Binet

test. Even though the screening

tests are age specific, the variance

would be expected to be greater

in a given group of children at an

earlier age. Also, some mothers

may have schooled their children

in several of the fine motor and

language tasks of the test that

are derived from the Stanford-

Binet test.

These limitations, in addition

to low r (^) values in the gross motor and (^) personal-social sec- tions would help to explain (^) the

relative superiority of combining

the fine motor and language sec-

tions as a predictor of a Stanford-

Binet intelligence score. Further,

to obviate the fact that we have

related this screening test,to only

one measure of intelligence (the

Stanford-Binet test), a followup

study of this cohort to examine

other parameters of social, emo-

tional, educational, and neuro-

logical function is planned for

REFERENCES (1) Collen, M. F., and Linden, C.: Screening in a group practice prepaid medical care plan. J

Chronic Dis 2: 400-408, Octo- ber 1955. (2) Frankenburg, W. F., and Dodds, J. B.: The Denver Developmen- tal Screening Test. J Pediatr 71: 181-191, August 1967. (3) Gesell, A., and Amatruda, C. S.: Developmental (^) diagnosis. Ed. 2. Harper & Row, New York, 1958. (4) Grant, J. A., and (^) Gittelsohn, A. M.: Estimating educational casualty from the (^1964) rubella epidemic in Maryland. Md State Med J. In press. (5) Hollingshead, A. B., and Red- lich, F. C.: Social class and men- tal illness. John Wiley & Sons, Inc., New York, 1958. (6) Thorner, R. M., and Remein, Q. R.: (^) Principles and procedures in the (^) evaluation of screening for (^) disease. PHS Publication No. 846, Public Health (^) Monogr No.

  1. U.S. Government (^) Printing Office, Washington, D.C., 1961. (7) Blum, H. L., Peters, H. (^) B., and Bettman, J. W.: The Orinda Study: Vision screening for ele- mentary schools. University of California Press, Los Angeles,

GRANT, JOHN A. (University of Maryland School of Medicine), and GITTELSOHN, ALAN M.:

The Denver Developmental Screening Test compared with the Stanford-Binet test: Evaluation

of 298 preschool children. Health Services Reports, Vol. 87, May 1972, pp 473-476.

The (^) Denver Developmental Screening Test (DDST) was evaluated by comparing the DDST performance of 298 (^) children with their perform- ance under the Stanford-Binet Test of Intelligence. The classic epidemiologic profile, including sensi- tivity (.788), specificity (.578), underreferral (.044), overreferral (.812) and tetrachoric r (.581) was used. Under these circumstances (testing of a dis- creet population at a specific point in time) the Denver Developmental Screening Test was shown to be a useful procedure for identifying preschool

children (^) with low Stanford-Binet intelligence scores, but it had a strong tendency to overrefer. Limitations of this evaluation were a 12-month interval between screening and reference tests

and the use of a broad spectrum test like the

Denver Developmental Screening Test to predict

a single parameter of development-intelligence

as measured by the Stanford-Binet test. The eval- uation does, however, reflect performance of the test under actual field conditions, (^) providing the administrator with valuable information for pro- gram control.

(^476) Health Services Reports